Chapter 8 - Group Process
THIS CHAPTER WILL DISCUSS:
1. What group discussion "functions" are.
2. How interactional researchers study group process.
3. Whether group process relates to group output.
4. Whether group discussion consists of a series of sequential states.
INTRODUCTION
In
Chapter 1 we examined the concept of "perspectives." As we explained,
scientists approach an object they wish to study with a particular viewpoint,
or perspective. Each perspective suggests distinct questions for the scientist.
Further,
with small-group research, it is best to approach different areas of study from
the viewpoints of particular perspectives. For example, we can study conflict
from any perspective; but to examine conflict in the most profitable way, we
based our discussion in this book on the relational perspective. Similarly, it
is perhaps best to approach topics such as conformity and deviance from the
structural viewpoint. Equally, much of what we covered in our chapter on
cohesiveness came from the two psychological perspectives.
In this
chapter, our topic is the process of communication in decision-making groups.
How can we best approach this topic? The theories and research studies that we
will describe relate to two viewpoints that are variants of perspectives that
we defined in Chapter 1.
The
first viewpoint, the functional approach, is a variant of the structural
perspective. We will use this approach when we discuss theories and studies
that relate to the functions that communication plays in group discussion.
The
second viewpoint, the interactional approach, is a
variant of the relational perspective. We will use this approach when we
examine theories and research that concern the way group discussion becomes
patterned in repetitive sequences over time.
In the
following sections, we will discuss many examples of scientific work on group
process, both from the functional and the interactional
approaches.
THE FUNCTIONAL APPROACH TO GROUP
PROCESS
The Concept of "Function"
Before
we discuss how communication functions during group discussion, let us look
again at the functional approach. In Chapter 1 we discussed the general claims
of functional theorists. We review those claims again now.
According
to functional theorists, any social system has a set of goals toward which it
directs its actions. Its primary goal is to survive and maintain itself. Beyond
this, each type of social system also has other goals particular to its type.
Decision-making
groups are a type of social system. As with any social system, a group has a
primary goal of maintenance. In addition, groups have the particular goal of
performing a task. These two goals are of utmost importance to groups. Their
importance has led to the central distinction, in this book, between task and
maintenance variables.
To reach
its goals, a social system must perform a set of necessary actions that
theorists have labeled "functions." In essence, the functional
perspective concerns the behaviors
of people in a social system.
What
about a group? How does it perform the functions it needs to reach its goals? A
group uses communicative statements. By talking with one another, group members
perform the necessary behaviors. In other words, a successful group discussion
is what takes a group to its goals.
For
example, a group is planning a party. The members need to use communicative
statements to perform their task. Statements such as "Should we hire a
band?", "We ought to serve pretzels and popcorn," and "So
we have decided to string ribbons around the room" all help the members
plan their party. Each time a person makes such a statement, he or she does an
action that helps the group toward its goal. In this way, members perform the
functions the group needs.
Positive and Negative Functions
The above
example statements help the group. Not all statements, however, serve positive
functions. Members can also make statements that serve negative functions and
that do not help. For example, the party-planning group may be plagued by one
negative member who keeps complaining about all the work he did for the last
party. He makes statements, such as, "Nobody ever thanks me for my
effort," "Who was it that made all those trips to the store?"
and "No way I'm gonna do all that again."
These statements also play a function in the group situation, a negative
function. Negative functions do not help the group and may prevent the group
from reaching its goals.
Types of Functions
It is
not enough simply to conclude that statements serve positive and negative functions
during group discussion. Scientists also want to be able to describe these
functions. In other words, a statement is not merely positive or negative; it
has other qualities. Does it summarize the discussion? Does it show agreement?
These kinds of questions help scientists find out what types of functions
exist.
More
generally, scientists want to be able to identify the kinds of positive and
negative functions that statements can have in a group. They want to categorize
the behaviors to understand them better. A scientist who has this knowledge can
observe a group's discussion and see if the members perform the positive
functions they need and avoid the negative functions they do not need. Based on
this observation, the scientist can predict whether the group will reach its
goals. Such a researcher could also recommend to group members the types of
statements they should and should not make to reach their goals.
Hence,
scientists are interested in the discussion functions that are possible in groups.
Several theorists have proposed lists of group discussion functions. We can
consider each of these as a theoretical view. The theory concerns the types of
functions groups need to perform to achieve their goals.
In the
following sections, we will describe three of these proposals.
Benne and Sheats's
Approach to Discussion Functions
"Functional Roles"
Benne
and Sheats (1948) formulated their list as a result
of what they had observed in groups. They did not present a list of discussion
functions per se but proposed a list of what they called "functional
roles." Benne and Sheats believed that they had
seen members play these "functional roles" in groups they had
observed. In essence, each member had taken on a kind of persona within the
group that related to the kinds of statements that person would tend to make.
Benne and Sheats labeled each functional role they
observed. For example, they might label a person who consistently tries to
lessen discord in a group as the "harmonizer."
Benne
and Sheats divided their list into three categories:
(1) group task roles, (2) group maintenance roles, and (3) individual roles.
Group task roles focus on the task at hand and include, for example, roles such
as the "elaborator," the "coordinator," and the "orienter." Group maintenance roles focus on keeping
the group together and include roles such as the "encourager," the
"harmonizer," and the "compromiser." Both task and
maintenance roles help the group pursue its goals.
Individual
roles are negative functional roles and do not help the group move toward its
goals. People playing these roles attempt to satisfy their own needs and
desires and work against the group as it tries to achieve its goals. Some
examples of individual roles include the "aggressor," the "dominator,"
and the "recognition seeker."
Accompanying Functions
Accompanying
each role is an analogous function. The name that Benne and Sheats
gave each role describes the kind of function that the person playing that role
would perform. For example, the "coordinator" would make statements
that provide the "coordination" function for the group.
As we
have stated, Benne and Sheats originally listed
functional roles. Some scientists and practitioners who have elaborated on
their original work have not focused on the roles that group members can play,
but on the functions themselves.
There
have been several such elaborations. Tables 8.1 and 8.2 show one example. These
lists are based on contributions from Professor Erma Jones of
Applications of Benne and Sheats's Approach
Which of
the task and maintenance roles on Benne and Sheats's
list are most critical for a group that wants to reach its goals? The scientists
make no overall claims in answer to this question nor do they make any
assertions about how often group members should play these roles.
We can
perhaps implicitly assume from their work that it is good for a group when the
members frequently take on task and maintenance roles. The more members play
roles that fall into these two categories, the more successful the group will
be. Benne and Sheats, however, do not discuss this
explicitly. They make few recommendations on how a group could successfully
utilize functional roles.
Table
8.1 |
Group
Task Functions |
|
Function |
Purpose |
Behaviors |
Initiating |
Give
direction and purpose to group |
Proposing
tasks and goals, defining problems, suggesting procedures and solutions |
Information
seeking |
Make
group aware of need for information |
Requesting
relevant facts, clarification |
Information
giving |
Provide
group with information relevant to its work |
Offering
relevant facts, avoiding reliance on opinion when facts needed |
Opinion
seeking |
Test
for consensus, find out group opinion |
Asking
for feelings or opinions |
Opinion
giving |
Provide
basis for group decision |
Stating
feelings or beliefs, evaluating a suggestion |
Clarifying |
Eliminate
confusion |
Defining
terms, interpreting ideas, indicating issues and alternatives |
Elaborating |
Reduce
ambiguity, show consequences of plans and positions |
Giving
examples, developing meanings, explaining |
Coordinating |
Adjust
issues or harmonize issues that may conflict |
Suggesting
ways to handle issues |
Procedure
developing |
Establish
an order to the meeting |
Suggesting
agenda, order of business |
Summarizing |
Show
how ideas are related, draw ideas together |
Pulling
together related issues, showing contradictions, restating suggestions,
offering conclusions |
Philosophizing |
Show
that a particular issue is not unique |
Drawing
general statements from specific ones, critically examining assumptions and
ideas |
Evaluating |
Keep
group in line with goals, provide sense of progress |
Measuring
accomplishments against goals, noting progress and blocks |
Testing
agreement |
Find
out how close group is to agreement |
Stating
ideas of agreement, asking if agreement is possible |
Energizing |
Keep
group working on problem |
Prodding
the members to action |
Orienting |
Keep
group in direction set by agenda |
Guiding
discussion by keeping group on track, moving discussion along |
Mediating |
Reconcile
disagreements |
Conciliating
differences, offering promises |
Table
8.2 |
Group
Maintenance Functions |
|
Function |
Purpose |
Behaviors |
Encouraging |
Bring
out others' opinions and give others recognition |
Speaking
positively to and of others, accepting others' contributions |
Expressing
feelings |
Call
group attention to own and others' ideas and reactions |
Expressing
own feelings and restating others' feelings and opinions |
Harmonizing |
Reduce
tension, allow group to express feelings |
Joking,
clowning, breaks and task-irrelevant statements |
Compromising |
Maintain
group cohesion |
Offering
or accepting compromises, yielding status, admitting error |
Facilitating |
Maintain
open discussion, keep channels open |
Drawing
out silent members, suggesting procedures for discussions |
Standard
setting |
Make
group aware of direction and progress |
Expressing
group concern, suggesting tasks, stating standards for group to achieve |
Interpreting |
Explain
what someone has said |
Paraphrasing
initial speaker |
Observing |
Making
group aware of its feelings |
Evaluating
the mood of the group |
Following |
Provide
stimulation and support for speakers |
Accepting
others' ideas, going along with group |
This
does not mean that Benne and Sheats make no observations
that relate to this topic. They have some relevant thoughts. One is that a
group would probably require different roles during different stages of its
discussion. For example, a group that is beginning to talk about its task would
probably have little need for an "evaluator." On the other hand, a
group
that
has progressed toward its decision would probably need someone to play that
role.
Benne
and Sheats point out that group
members should not limit the range of roles they can play. In a group with
a rigid role structure, each member plays only one or two roles during the
whole discussion. Such a group will probably not be as successful as a group
that has a more flexible structure. A flexible structure would allow everyone
to play many roles. This kind of system helps a group effectively utilize the
talents of its members.
Bales's Approach to Discussion Functions
The "Equilibrium Problem"
Unlike
Benne and Sheats, Bales (1950, 1953) used a
well-developed theory of group process to develop his list of discussion
functions. The theory he used is a good example of the functional perspective.
Two
Conflicting Goals
As you
will remember, functionalists theorize that groups try to achieve two major
goals: maintain the group and perform the group's task. To achieve each goal,
the group must perform certain actions.
For
example, a group has the task of cleaning a room quickly. The
demands of the task equire certain actions.
For instance, the group must be strictly organized and work hard. On the other
hand, the group cannot deny its other goal, maintaining itself as a group. This
goal also requires certain actions. For instance, members may need to joke with
one another and to spend time forming friendships. As you can see, the cleaning
group has task and maintenance demands that are in conflict. In fact, the
demands oppose each other.
Bales
believed that it is inevitable that task and maintenance demands oppose each
other, which leads to a major problem: Task performance usually results in
maintenance problems. For example, if the group cleans the room quickly, it
probably doesn't give itself enough time for the necessary social interaction.
As the group tries to deal with these maintenance difficulties, more task
dilemmas arise. If the cleaning group takes time to allow members to form
friendships, it probably is not cleaning the room as quickly as it could.
Task Changes and Maintenance
Problems
Further,
changes in a group's relationship with its task will cause maintenance
problems. For example, a group is reassigned from a coordination task to an
accuracy task. This change requires adjusting the group's structure. Changes
will include altering its division of labor and its power hierarchy. As we have
seen, faced with a coordination task, a group relies equally on all members to
perform their part of the task competently. Faced with an accuracy task,
however, the group should rely on only those members most competent to perform
the task. Hence, the group probably will change from one in which members share
responsibility and power to one in which the most competent members hold
responsibility and power.
The
change to a power hierarchy will mean improved task performance. The group's
maintenance structure is disturbed, however. We would expect that the members
who suddenly find themselves peripheral to the task will lose satisfaction in
their jobs. This will lead to losses in cohesiveness, which could threaten the
group's survival. When the group realizes that it has lower morale, it will try
to rebuild cohesiveness by changing its social structure. Once again it will
give more responsibility to the peripheral members, leading to a redefinition
of task roles. Ultimately, the group might return to a shared power structure.
If it did, it would not be able to perform the accuracy task as well as it
could using a power hierarchy.
The
danger is that a group will swing back and forth between task and maintenance
crises. Bales called this the "equilibrium problem," because a group
can manage the problem by developing properties that serve to balance these
extremes. It can do so by repeating sequences of actions that alternately
dampen task and maintenance disturbances. By carefully tracking both sets of
problems, the group can maintain its balance.
Sequential Task Problems
As we
have seen, the first goal of a group is to maintain itself. The second goal is
to perform its task. Functionalists believe that communication performs a
special role in task performance. Van Lear (1996) has described this belief:
1 -
Group decisions do not automatically occur. They must be developed.
2 -
Communication and its associated functions are necessary for a developed group
decision.
3 -
Decisions tend to develop through a series of stages or phases. This is because certain
communicative functions are needed to move the group from one stage to the
next.
The task
of the theorist is to determine what the phases of group task performance are,
and how communication functions to help the group complete each phase and move
on to the next.
In Bales's view, successful task performance rests on the
group's ability to solve three problems: orientation, evaluation, and control. Orientation involves the members
coming to a common understanding and definition of the task. For example,
members in the cleaning group should all understand that their task is to
neaten a room.
Evaluation involves members' developing
common values regarding what a good solution must accomplish. For example,
should the room be so clean that it sparkles when the group is finished, or
will a relatively clean room be a good solution? Control involves the members' finding the best solution
for the task, using the power and influence relationships among its members.
Looking at the cleaning group, who should sweep the room and who is good at
polishing?
As you
can see, the group must solve the three problems sequentially. Each requires
successfully solving the previous problem. For example, if the group decides
that the room doesn't need to really sparkle, it is not necessary for the
best-qualified people to do the jobs that they do. well.
Any group member could probably do a fair job; thus, the problem of control
changes.
Bales's Research Methodology
As you
can recall, according to functional theory, each group has a necessary set of
functions--actions that a group must perform to reach its goals and solve its
problems. In turn, communicative statements are the actions that groups can use
to perform these necessary functions. It follows from this that Bales would
measure group communication to evaluate his claims.
How did
Bales measure this communication? To create a methodology he proposed 12
functions. Bales believed that these functions are critical for groups as they
work to maintain equilibrium and solve their orientation, evaluation, and
control problems. Bales used his list of functions to perform content analyses
of the communication that occurred during the group meetings he studied.
Observers "coded" the group's communication in terms of the functions
ir performed. Hence, we will use the term
"coding scheme" to describe a list such as he created.
Table
8.3 illustrates his coding scheme.
Table
8.3 |
Bales's Interaction Process Analysis coding scheme |
Categories |
Specific
Examples |
Positive
Reactions |
|
1.
Shows solidarity |
Jokes,
gives help, rewards others |
2.
Shows tension release |
Laughs,
shows satisfaction |
3.
Shows agreement |
Understands,
concurs, complies, passively accepts |
Attempted
Answers |
|
4.
Gives suggestion |
Directs,
proposes, controls |
5.
Gives opinion |
Evaluates,
analyzes, expresses feelings or wishes |
6.
Gives information |
Orients,
repeats, clarifies, confirms |
Questions |
|
7.
Asks for information |
Requests
orientation, repetition, confirmation, clarification |
8.
Asks for opinion |
Requests
evaluation, analysis, expression of feeling or wishes |
9.
Asks for suggestion |
Requests
directions, proposals |
Negative
Reactions |
|
10.
Shows disagreement |
Passively
rejects, resorts to formality, withholds help |
11.
Shows tension |
Asks
for help, withdraws |
12.
Shows antagonism |
Deflates
others, defends or asserts self |
As you
can see, the last six functions are "mirror images" of the first six.
The first three and the last three functions relate directly to one another.
All have to do with group maintenance. Functions 1, 2, and 3 are "positive
reactions." Bales believed a group needs these functions to maintain its
cohesiveness. Functions 10, 11, and 12 mirror them and are their opposites.
They are "negative reactions" that endanger group cohesiveness. For
instance, "shows solidarity" is a positive reaction; "shows
antagonism" is a negative reaction.
The
other six functions relate to the group's goal of completing its task. They
still mirror one another, however. They are, in a sense, complementary. One set
of behaviors asks questions; the other set attempts answers. The
"questions" in functions 7, 8, and 9 mirror the "attempted
answers" that are functions 4, 5, and 6.
In
general, Bales presented a wealth of findings from his studies of groups. As
you can recall, in Chapter 2 we discussed his finding that the differences in
group members' talk time increases as group size increases. More specifically,
Bales used his coding scheme to test his hypotheses about group equilibrium and
about the problems of group orientation, evaluation, and control. Later in this
chapter we will discuss these findings in some detail.
For the
present, it is sufficient to refer to one of Bales's
overall claims that group functioning is not so much a result of having many
"good" functions as it is a result of using those "good"
functions properly. Benne and Sheats may have
proposed that a successful group has a high quantity of task and maintenance
functions. Bales asserted that this was not quite the case. He claimed instead
it is more important that groups properly balance task and maintenance
statements. In other words, Bales believed that groups seem to do well when
these statements are in a certain proportion and occur in a certain sequence.
Hirokawa's Approach to Discussion Functions
Essential
Functions
Hirokawa
(1982) claimed that scholars should relate group task performance to
communicative functions that are essential for the group's task. Essential
functions are those that groups must perform to complete their tasks successfully.
Hirokawa
argued that not all earlier functional approaches had focused on these
essential communicative functions. Hirokawa singled
out Bales's theory for criticism. As you can recall, Bales's coding scheme includes functions such as "asks
for opinions," a phrase that does not specify an essential function. Hirokawa said that, under this heading, group members can
ask all kinds of questions that might or might not relate to the group's task.
For
instance, a member can ask a question such as, "What opinions do you have
regarding the proposals we've made so far as we've looked at the decision we
have to make?" This action is relevant to the group's task. As such, it is
an essential function. In contrast, another member could interrupt the group's
task work to ask, "Where should we go to dinner after our meeting?"
This action is not relevant to the group's task. Therefore, according to Hirokawa, it is not essential. Nevertheless, Bales would
count both questions in his coding scheme and say that both perform equally
well as functions under the heading of "asks for opinions."
With all
this in mind, Hirokawa created his own list of
functions that he believed clearly related to groups' tasks. Hirokawa looked at earlier work on this topic and concluded
that four functions meet his requirements:
1.
Establishment of operating procedures
2.
Analysis of possible solutions
3.
Generation of possible solutions
4.
Evaluation of possible solutions
Similarity to Other Lists
Unfortunately,
Hirokawa's list has the same problem that he believed
Bales's list had. The categories can contain a
statement that does not relate to a group's task. Members can perform a
function from this list that is irrelevant to a task just as easily as they can
perform a function that is relevant.
We can
go back to Hirokawa's own example to see how this
could happen. A group member could easily "generate a solution" to
the non-task issue of where the group members should go to dinner after the
meeting. Hence, Hirokawa was not able successfully to
isolate the essential task functions. This problem does not, however, in any
way invalidate his list.
Research Questions
At the time of
his essay, in 1982, Hirokawa was not sure exactly how
best to relate group task performance to the functions on his list. Is the
number of functions or the sequence in which they occur more important? Which
relates more to how well a group does?
As we
will discuss later in this chapter, Hirokawa went on
to study these questions.
The Three Levels of Discussion
Functions
Many
Proposals to Utilize
We have
just discussed three proposals that look at communicative statements within
group discussion. Each attempted to describe the types of functions that those
statements can perform.
What can
we do with all these proposals? When we see a statement, how should we analyze
it? Do we have to pick and choose from among the proposals, or can we combine
them?
For
example, someone makes the following statement during a group discussion:
"Several corporations have adopted the second proposal." How would
this statement fit into each proposal we have described? Benne and Sheats might have decided that it serves the function of
"elaborating." On the other hand, Bales might have considered that it
performed the function of "giving information." In contrast, Hirokawa could have claimed that the statement fulfilled
the function of "evaluating a proposal."
In
addition, each theory is specific to the relationship between discussion
functions and a group's performance. Which formula is most likely to ensure
success? What might make a group fail? Benne and Sheats
would probably have claimed that how well a group does depends on the number of
functions its members perform. Bales, instead, would have said that a group's performance
depends on either the proportion or the sequence of functions that its members
act out.
Method to Blend Proposals
Can all
of these ideas be correct? Can one statement, such as our example above,
perform many types of functions? If yes, can these functions relate to one
another in any way? Finally, can functions interact with group performance in
all of these ways?
The
answer to all three questions is yes. One method can utilize all these
proposals simultaneously.
Based
on Philosophical Studies of Actions
To
understand this method, let us offer an example. You are unhappily watching a
comedian perform at a supper club. You decide you want to see the comedian
leave the stage. You believe that the comedian may leave if you register
displeasure. To show this displeasure, you decide to throw a dinner roll at the
comedian. You know how to do this concrete action. All you need to do is to
pick up the roll, move your arm forward quickly, and let go of the roll at the
proper moment.
Philosophers
interested in human action have studied such situations. Essentially, someone
watching you would only see you throw a dinner roll. Philosophers would say
that in reality you have simultaneously performed at least four action. All four actions relate to one another. How?
Levels
of Abstraction
The
actions relate through levels of abstraction. Each is on a different level.
Some are more abstract; others are less so. In essence, the more abstract the
action, the more involved the thinking is at that level of action.
For
instance, the action of "throwing a dinner roll" is more abstract
than the action of "moving your arm forward." Someone who saw your
arm move could say with some certainty that you moved your arm forward. If the
person wants to know more about why you moved your arm forward, the situation
is more complex. If the person moves to a higher level of abstraction, he or
she could discover that the action of deciding to throw a dinner roll led you
to move your arm.
Relationships
Between Levels
Further,
the levels of abstraction relate in two ways: the "in-order-to"
relationship and the "by-means-of" relationship. It is as if the
levels were on a ladder. As you move up each rung of the ladder, you seek the
motivating action behind the action on your current rung. As you move down the
ladder, you seek the actions behind the motivating action on your rung. In
other words, you move up to answer the question of why, and you move down to
answer the question of how. If someone asks why, you can begin your answer with
"In order to ..." If someone asks how, you
can answer with "By means of...."
When
philosophers look at one action and move up to the more abstract action related
to it, they see an "in-order-to" relationship. They see an action
that is rather concrete, such as an arm that moves forward. To discover the
less concrete action behind it, they can ask themselves, "Someone
performed this action in order to do what?" For example, you moved your
arm forward "in order to" throw the dinner roll.
When
philosophers want to move the other way, from more abstract to less abstract
actions, they see a "by-means-of" relationship. People act out their
less concrete actions by means of more concrete ones. For example, you wanted
to register your displeasure. This desire was a rather abstract action. You
acted out this abstract action "by means of" throwing a dinner roll.
The
"act-tree" in Figure 8.1 diagrams the relationships among these
actions (Goldman, 1970).
FIGURE
8.1
Thus, we
can "describe" the same physical action in many ways. In this case,
the physical action is the forward movement of an arm. Figure 8.1 shows that the arm movement did four things. It moved
forward, threw a dinner roll, registered displeasure, and convinced a comedian
to leave a stage. Each action is on a different level of abstraction, but all
are related. Each level connects to the one above it through an
"in-order-to" relationship and to the one below it through a
"by-means-of" relationship.
The
relationships among levels are flexible. A person can register displeasure
"by-means-of" many lower level actions other than throwing a dinner
roll. Similarly, someone can throw a dinner roll "in-order-to"
perform many higher level actions that have nothing to do with registering
displeasure.
Application of Method to
Communication Functions
Now that
we have examined the ideas behind the "act-tree," we can discuss how
we can use this method to look at communication functions.
Functions
Are Actions
Communication
functions are a type of action. Therefore, we can analyze them in the same way
that we analyze other actions. As can any other action, a statement that a
group member makes can simultaneously serve several functions. In essence,
there are many answers to the question of why a group member said something, in the same way that there were many reasons you
moved your arm as you sat in a dinner theater.
Similarly,
each function of a statement can reside on different levels of abstraction.
Every level relates to a more abstract level via "in-order-to"
relationships and to less abstract levels via "by-means-of"
relationships.
The
"Act-Tree" and the Three Proposals
Now let
us go back to our example statement and analyze it using the
"act-tree." We see that the sentence, "Several corporations have
adopted the second proposal," can simultaneously serve many functions. It
can simultaneously serve all the functions that we said the three proposals
claimed. We find that the three proposals--from Benne and Sheats,
Bales, and Hirokawa--all present claims that can work
together.
We
propose that as a representation of what takes place, the act-tree in Figure
8.2 may be defensible:
FIGURE
8.2
As we
can see, the statement "Several corporations have adopted..."
simultaneously serves functions at different levels. At the highest level of
this diagram, the statement evaluates a proposal. At the lowest level, it gives
information, and so on.
The
functions themselves remain on their levels. For instance, elaborating will
always be a higher level function than simply giving information. The
relationships among these functions are flexible, however. Many functions are
on each level. For example, a person can give information "in order
to" perform a middle-level function other than elaborating. Similarly,
someone can elaborate "by means of" a low-level function that is not
giving information.
The
"Act-Tree" and Group Performance
Because
of this flexibility, functions on different levels can relate to group
performance in different ways. For example, in one group many statements may
serve the function of elaborating, but they do so by means of several lower
level functions. Hence, if we code the functions that relate to how well that
group does, we would find several lower level functions at work, but only one
middle-level one. We could find an infinite number of such combinations.
With
this in mind, we can examine again the theories of how communicative functions
relate to group performance. We can now look at the functions on different
levels. It is conceivable that we can link low-level functions to group
performance through their proportion, which is what Bales believed. It is also
possible that middle-level functions relate to group performance through sheer
number, as Benne and Sheats's approach may imply.
Hence, both theories may be valid at the same time because the functions exist
on different levels.
Conclusions
The
moral of the story is that the levels of functions are important. Theorists who
advocate the functional approach to group discussion need to remember this.
When they make claims about group discussion, they must take care not to mix
levels of abstraction. Further, they cannot assume that the relationship
between functions on different levels is inflexible. Finally, these theorists
must not believe they can relate group performance to all functions in the same
way. They need to be careful about which level of the functions they wish to
relate to group performance. Functions from different levels perform
differently, and any theory about group performance must account for this.
Later in
this chapter we will return to the functional approach. We will discuss the
results of functionalist research into the process of group discussion. In the
meantime, however, we will turn to the second approach--how interactional
theorists look at group process.
THE INTERACTIONAL APPROACH TO
GROUP PROCESS
As we
mentioned in Chapter 1, and again earlier in this chapter, advocates of the interactional approach look at group patterns that form
over time. They see groups as aggregates in which, over time, repetitive
communication sequences become patterned. As a consequence, group process is of
utmost importance to them. They wish to discover the processes that lead to
patterns. Scientists who have adopted the interactional
approach focus their efforts on the study of group process.
The
theoretical underpinnings to account for this focus are in an essay by B. A.
Fisher and Hawes (1971). They distinguished between two approaches to studying
small groups--the Human Systems Model, or "HSM," and the Interact
Systems Model, or "ISM."
A
"system" exists, technically speaking, when a number of objects are
interrelated. The relationship of the parts is such that the action of one
object affects the action of all other objects in the system. Both the
relational and structural perspectives, including all their variants, view a
small group as such a system. This idea differentiates them from the
psychological perspective. A scientist from the structural or the relational
perspective is more concerned with the system as a whole than with its
individual parts. The interactionalists agree with
this view.
Human Systems Model (HSM)
Most
variants assume that the Human Systems Model (HSM) applies to groups. This
model holds that the "objects" in a system are "people."
HSM theorists characterize the systems they study by the relationships among
the people. These relationships include variables such as the degree of
cohesiveness and the power relations among group members.
Interact Systems Model (ISM)
The ISM
(Interact Systems Model) derives from the extreme interactional
view. In contrast to the HSM, this model holds that "units of
communication" are the objects that interrelate in a system. Theorists
using the ISM call the units of communication "acts." They
characterize the systems they study by referring to recurring patterns of these
acts. In so doing, they examine which pattern of interaction leads to another
pattern, and so on.
The ISM
extreme interactionalists believe that the basic unit
of analysis is not the unit of a dyad but rather a system of two
"acts," the interact.
The acts are conversational utterances. To be part of an
interact, the acts must be contiguous, that is side-by-side in time, one
occurring immediately after the other. The acts must also be probabilistically
related.
What
does it mean to be probabilistically related? Let us consider Bales's coding scheme as an example. As we described above,
this coding scheme includes categories such as "Gives suggestion."
Now let us imagine that you were to perform a content analysis of a group
discussion using Bales's coding scheme. How often
would group members give suggestions? Based on Bales's
(1953) research, we might imagine that about five percent of members'
utterances would fit in this category.
An interactionalist would not be satisfied with this
information. An interactionalist would want to know
the circumstances under which a group member is likely to give a suggestion.
Once again looking at Bales's data, we find that when
one member makes an utterance that is coded "Asks for suggestion,"
the odds are 36 percent that another person will respond by giving one. Looking
at these two pieces of data together, it seems that giving a suggestion is a
relatively rare event overall,
but if someone has just asked for one, giving a suggestion is fairly likely to
occur. Asking for and giving suggestions are then probabilistically related.
Example of Interactional
Perspective Research
The goal
of interactionalists when they research group process
is to determine whether the utterances in group discussion are
probabilistically related. If so, then we can say that group discussion has a
"sequential structure." Baird's (1974) research is a good example.
Baird compared the interaction of eight groups of four or five members each.
Baird gave the first four groups "cooperative" instructions. He told
members that he would grade their performance as a group. The second four
groups received "competitive" instructions. Baird told participants
that their grades would be based on their relative individual performances.
Results
showed that competitive group members tended to initiate and disagree
more than cooperative group members. Also, persons in the competitive groups
informed one another and agreed less than the members of the other four groups.
We can see the results in Table 8.4 with numbers representing the proportions
of acts in each category.
Table
8.4 |
Baird's
Data |
|
Category |
Cooperative
Groups |
Competitive
Groups |
1 -
gives information |
.540 |
.466 |
2 -
expresses agreement |
.177 |
.148 |
3 -
asks for information |
.132 |
.113 |
4 -
initiates and develops theme |
.085 |
.163 |
5 -
expresses disagreement+ |
.066 |
.110 |
The
differences between cooperative and competitive groups also affected the
sequential structure of the interaction of the groups. Before we discuss these
results, we offer a note to help in the interpretation of the data. Remember
from our example above that suggestions occur about
five percent overall but more than a third of the time given that someone has
just asked for one. We need to interpret this data the same way. For example,
if cooperative groups give information 54 percent of the time, then there is
sequential structure in the group discussion if giving information follows
another communication act more or less than 54 percent of the time. If giving
information follows another act more than 54 percent of the time, we will say
that it occurs more often "than expected." Similarly, if giving
information follows another act less than 54 percent of the time, we will say
that it occurs less often "than expected."
The
diagrams in Figures 8.3 and 8.4 use the categories from Baird's research that
we listed in Table 8.4. They show Baird's most significant findings regarding
how often interacts in his groups varied from what we would expect.
FIGURE
8.3
FIGURE
8.4
Note
that some interacts occurred more often than acts in both cooperative and
competitive groups. For example, "initiates and develops theme" was
followed by "asks for information" more often than expected.
Similarly, "asks for information" was followed by "gives
information" more often than expected. We can conclude that these
interacts play an important function in any group discussion.
Other
interacts differed between the groups. Some of the disparities are unexpected.
For example, "initiates and develops theme" both followed and is
followed by "expresses agreement" more often in competitive
situations than in cooperative ones. Some differences are more predictable. In
competitive groups, "expresses disagreement" was followed by
"expresses agreement" less often than by chance, a sign that conflict
was occurring. In contrast, in cooperative groups, "expresses
agreement" was followed by "expresses disagreement" less often
than by chance, a sign that conflict was not occurring. In other words,
conflict existed more often in competitive situations than in cooperative
settings.
Conclusions
During the 1970s, ISM modelers produced a great deal of research into group
process. However, as a whole we did not learn a lot from these studies. There
are several reasons for this failure. One of these reasons was that most ISM
scientists believed that by just looking at their data, they could eventually
come up with theories. Researchers outside the ISM realm, such as Bales, often
work differently. They prefer to start with a theory that, in
effect, tell them what to look for in interaction. They then test their
theory to see if the research supports it.
This
approach is not perfect. A theory can, for example, keep researchers from
seeing patterns in their data that may be important but that are irrelevant to
their theory. It is also true, however, that without a theory researchers may
not be able to find any patterns. ISM modelers appeared to have been merely
looking at their data. If instead they had looked for something in particular,
they might have found it.
Second,
there is a problem even if ISM researchers had been able to produce theories
about group interaction. These theories would not have told us anything
particularly useful. The ISM modelers were willing to study how HSM input
variables affect group process. They were uninterested in describing the
effects of process, in turn, on HSM output variables. However, the
process-output relationship is just as important as the input-process linkage.
Task groups do not talk simply in order to talk. They talk so that they can
perform a task, leading to output variables. By ignoring the impact of process
on output, ISM researchers made it impossible to learn how process can be used
to help groups perform their task or get along with one another better from
their perspective. How can this problem be solved? By
studying the relationship between group interaction and group output.
This is what researchers such as Bales and Hirokawa
have done.
For the
remainder of this chapter we will discuss group-process research related to
potentially useful issues. These studies have attempted to answer two general
questions: First, does group process relate to group output? And second, can
scientists divide the process of group discussion into a series of phases? We
will discuss each question in turn. The studies we will describe refer to ideas
already introduced in this chapter.
THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN GROUP
PROCESS AND GROUP OUTPUT
Bales
Earlier
in this chapter, we described Bales's belief that
groups have an inherent problem balancing two opposing goals: group maintenance
and task performance. Each goal places opposing demands on a group. Thus,
groups have an "equilibrium problem."
Bales
believed that groups use communication in different ways to perform this
balancing act. Group output depends upon how well they do it. Therefore, he
theorized that a researcher should examine a group's communication to see the
functions it plays during discussion. By doing this, the researcher can see how
successfully the group solves its "equilibrium problem." Bales
proposed a coding scheme with 12 categories to use when he content analyzed
group discussion. We discussed this coding scheme earlier in this chapter.
During
the late 1940s and early 1950s, Bales performed a great deal of research on
group discussion. We will now look closely at this research, starting with a
summary of how he performed his studies.
Method
Bales
(1953) brought together groups of three to eight people. The groups met to
discuss what Bales called "human relations cases." The researchers
gave members five-page summaries relating the difficulties of a fictional
administrator in dealing with subordinates. The group's decision-making task was
to consider why these difficulties arose. In addition, they were to decide what
could be done to solve them. The researchers gave the groups 40 minutes to
perform the task. When they finished, members rated their reactions to the
discussion. They also noted their level of satisfaction with the group, their
views of the other group members, and their opinions of the group as a whole.
As the
groups worked, observers behind one-way mirrors classified every communicative
act according to three criteria: who said it, to whom it was said (for example,
to an individual member or to the group as a whole), and the act's function.
Research Results
Acts. Overall, in Bales's groups, an average of 25 percent of a group's statements were
positive reactions. Only 11 percent were negative. Bales believed that negative
reactions indicated maintenance disturbances. Positive statements needed to
outnumber the negative to restore equilibrium. For example, a person might need
to give two or three compliments to make up for one harsh word.
Indeed,
in one example of a satisfied group, positive reactions were 34 percent of the
total; negative reactions were only 5 percent. In contrast, one dissatisfied group had only 16 percent positive statements
and 17 percent negative. As we can see, the more satisfied group made a far
greater proportion of positive statements as compared with negative statements
than did the less contented group. Also, overall the more satisfied group
performed more maintenance acts, or statements classified as negative or
positive.
In
task-oriented discussion, the groups had an average of 57 percent attempted
answers. Only 7 percent were questions. Satisfaction levels were unrelated to
the number of attempted answers. Dissatisfied groups, however, did ask
relatively more questions than contented groups (10 percent versus 4 percent).
Overall, in dissatisfied groups the number of
questions increased at the expense of maintenance, negative or positive,
statements. Less satisfied group members seemed to spend more time trying to
understand their tasks than they did trying to get to know one another.
Interacts. Groups deal with the problem of equilibrium at the level
of interacts. Remember, Bales hypothesized that a group needs to balance the
opposing pressures of task and maintenance needs. He believed that groups could
do this by alternating between task statements and maintenance statements.
Several research results speak to this hypothesis.
For
example, Bales saw that, when a group member reacts positively or negatively,
he or she is likely to continue with an attempted answer. Any further
continuation is also likely to be an attempted answer. The diagram in Figure
8.5 illustrates this relationship between contiguous communication acts by the
same person.
People
dampen their own maintenance reactions by using attempted answers. They try to
tell why they feel as they do. For example, a group participant might express
the negative reaction, "It seems as if we don't use our time very
well," and then continue with an attempted answer that helps to soften the
negative words, such as, "Maybe this is because we aren't as organized as
we could be." After saying this, the person will probably yield the floor
to another member, which also helps to mitigate the negative statement.
FIGURE
8.5
FIGURE
8.6
Contiguous
statements can also come from different people. In this case, positive and
negative reactions are also balanced by attempted answers, as the diagram in
Figure 8.6 shows.
As you
can see, attempted answers in this scenario lead to statements of agreement.
For example, Amy says, "I think we need to focus more on our work."
Bill continues the conversation by attempting to answer Ann's negative
statement. He says, "Maybe we need an outline." Ken agrees, saying,
"An outline is a good idea, Bill. I've worked in other groups who did
that." Such a conversation pattern is consistent with theory. Theory holds
that task-oriented activity seems to build up tensions that are broken by
positive maintenance acts. When the tension started to build as Bill talked
about an outline, Ken's positive words helped to ease the tension.
In the
diagrams above we omitted links that were either smaller than .1 or irrelevant
to the equilibrium hypothesis. One strong regularity
that we left out is worth mentioning, however. Attempted answers tend to follow
questions. This is not surprising, particularly when contiguous statements come
from different group members. Bill might ask, "Does anyone know how to
make an outline?" and Ken may continue with the attempted answer of,
"I think I do. Let's get some paper."
Conclusions
In
summary, Bales was able to show through his research that groups use
communication to solve the equilibrium problem. A group builds up tensions when
it performs task functions. Positive reactions dissipate these tensions. The
process that groups use to dissipate tensions takes place on two levels: acts
and interacts.
On the
level of acts, Bales claimed that satisfied groups make many more positive
reactions than negative ones. In these groups, members use positive reactions
to bleed off the tension that comes through task work and negative reactions.
The positive messages help restore balance. In contrast, dissatisfied groups do
not have such a preponderance of positive reactions. Without these positive
reactions they are unable to bleed off the tension successfully.
On the
level of interacts, Bales was able to see in detail the process groups use to
dissipate tension. In a satisfied group, the process on this level often
follows a certain pattern. First, tension builds when members make negative
reactions. Second, to begin to lessen the tension, members often use an
attempted answer. Finally, when one member attempts an answer, another member
will often respond with a positive reaction. This circle of messages completes
the dampening process. Afterward, the group is ready to return to task work,
using questions or attempted answers.
Thus,
both Bales's theory and his research findings imply
that a successful group's output is based on the proper proportion and sequence
of communicative functions. Group members need to know how to balance and
coordinate their statements if they want their group to do well.
Of
course, keep in mind that Bales worked with functions that are on a low level
of abstraction. His findings support his ideas only as far as those particular
functions are concerned. What if one were to examine discussion functions at a
different level of abstraction? Such an examination could reveal a different
relationship between group process and group performance.
Hirokawa
As we discussed earlier, Hirokawa (1982) focused on
communicative functions on a high level of abstraction. His belief was that
scientists should relate group performance to these high-level functions. In
the decade after that proposal, Hirokawa performed a
series of studies to find out which functions are essential for successful
groups. In other words, which high-level communicative functions must groups
perform to reach high-quality decisions? In Hirokawa's
studies, he asked groups to make decisions about policy issues including topics
such as traffic safety and plagiarism. He then brought in experts on these
topics to judge the quality of each group's decision. Finally, he analyzed the
group discussions to see which high-level communicative functions each group
performed. He wanted to find out the relationship between these functions and
the quality of each group's decision.
Method
Hirokawa
used several methods to analyze group discussion. Before we discuss his results,
we need to understand the shortcomings of one of his methods. In this method, Hirokawa trained people to observe groups and look for sets
of possible functions. The observers watched videotapes of group discussions
and then judged the extent to which the groups performed each set of functions.
A serious problem arises because of halo and horns effects.
As you
can recall, in Chapter 3 we discussed halo and horns effects. These effects take place, for example, when a research participant
sees a picture of an attractive man and, without knowing anything more about
him, judges him intelligent and exciting as well as attractive. This
happens because many people believe that these three attributes--attractive,
intelligent, and exciting--usually "go together." Such a belief
affects the participant's judgment of the attractive person.
This
technique of Hirokawa's is problematic because it
relies on judgments from observers. We have good reason to believe that
observers, despite their training, are vulnerable to the halo and horns
effects. For instance, observers are likely to believe that a "good"
group will perform "good" functions. Similarly, they probably think
that a "bad" group will not perform good functions. The process could
start when they see a group do a good function. After they see this, they
probably will label that group as "good." Once that happens, they are
likely to expect the group to do well, and they will judge that it performs all
of the good functions.
Studies
indicate that this happens. For example, in one study (Hirokawa,
1988, Study 1) observers strongly tended to look at the functions they were
judging and to decide that groups had performed all these functions to about
the same extent. Thus, a method that relies on observers' ratings does not
result in trustworthy findings.
How then
should scientists measure which functions occur in group discussion? The most
valid method is the one that Bales used. Coders should observe group discussion
and, using a coding scheme, note when functions actually occur.
Hirokawa
did use this technique in some of his studies. For example, he often used a
coding scheme when he studied the actual discussions of groups. His coding
scheme consisted of high-level communicative functions (for example, Hirokawa, 1988, Study 2). The studies that used a coding
scheme have more trustworthy findings than the studies that used observers'
ratings.
Conclusions
Hirokawa
concluded from his research that groups need a small set of "critical
functions" to make successful decisions. Hirokawa
has presented various lists in his essays. The following five functions have
recurred regularly:
1. The
group must come to understand the nature of the dilemma they face.
2. The
group must agree on the requirements for an acceptable solution.
3. The
group must come up with a range of realistic alternative proposals for
solutions.
4. The
group needs to thoroughly and accurately assess the positive consequences of
each alternative proposal.
5. The
group must thoroughly and accurately assess the negative consequences of each
alternative proposal.
Hirokawa's research has led him to conclude that when communicative statements
help groups meet these requirements, they bring about a high-quality decision.
Hewes's Challenge
Hewes (1986) challenged the conclusions of scientists
such as Bales and Hirokawa. He did not agree that
group discussion affects group output.
Implications of the
Input-Process-Output Model
To
present his challenge, Hewes utilized the
input-process-output model of group discussion. He considered some implications
of the model to help him with his work. As we discussed in Chapter 1, the
input-process-output model diagrams what we should think about when we study
group discussion.
As the
model shows, a scientist needs to keep a number of things in mind. For
instance, to understand group discussion, a scientist needs to think about the
variables that precede it and to look at how such variables affect the process
of group discussion itself. These input variables include the size of the
group, the group's task, and the amount of cohesiveness among its members. A
scientist then also needs to consider how the process of discussion affects
group output variables. A researcher will understand how group discussion works
only if he or she simultaneously considers all these variables and their
relationships.
Using
this model, Hewes approached the question of how
group process affects group output by focusing on input variables. He proposed
that these variables are the most important factor as far as group output is
concerned. Therefore, group process loses its significance. Hewes
argued that, if a scientist wants to show that communication truly affects
group output, he or she must show that the effects of communication go over and
above the effects of input variables.
The
general argument is that the group members' initial qualities, not what they
discuss when they come together, most affect their group's output. This general
argument has already come up several times in this book. One significant
example appeared in Chapter 2, and another was in Chapter 7.
Examples of General Argument
In
Chapter 2 we asked if people should perform tasks individually or in groups. We
often showed that the answer depended on what affects decision accuracy. Did
interaction among group members lead to the output, or was output instead an
aggregation of members' work? The Lorge/Solomon Model
A looked at accuracy tasks. It was based on the assumption that interaction has
no effect on accuracy when groups work on these kinds of tasks. Instead, Model
A predicts a group's accuracy by looking at the members individually. The
prediction is based on the odds that members can solve the problem
individually. Thus, Model A directly relates an input variable to an output
variable. The input variable consists of the group members' individual
knowledge and skill. The output variable is the group's accuracy.
Social
decision schemes work the same way for quality tasks. They begin with each
group member's prediscussional opinion. They then
predict how these opinions will be "combined" into the group's
decision. It likewise presumes that interaction has no impact on this
combination.
In
Chapter 7 we discussed this general argument again when we looked at social
influence. We examined five positions concerning how social influence works in
groups. In the first four, scientists believed that they could predict a
group's decision by looking at the members' prediscussional
ideas. Prediscussional ideas comprise an input variable, and the group's
decision is its output.
Position
1 is based on social decision schemes. Under Position 2, we examined social
comparison theory, which explains the group polarization effect by
hypothesizing that members affect one another by sharing their prediscussional opinions. The persuasive arguments theory
from Position 3 differs a bit from the first two positions; however, it also
emphasizes prediscussional ideas. Someone can predict
a group's decision by looking at the arguments that members have before they
talk with one another. In every case, as you can see, these proposals
hypothesize that an input variable sufficiently accounts for group output. Only
Position 5 differs. Its structurational approach
claims that group discussion affects group output and that this effect is over
and above the effect of group input.
Hence,
many theories incorporate Hewes's general idea. His
specific proposal challenges researchers. He believes that a scientist who
wishes to claim that group process really affects group output must show that
the effects of group process are independent of the effects of input variables.
Group process must do something special that input variables alone do not do.
When Hewes wrote this essay, either
Bales nor Hirokawa had considered this issue.
Possible inadequacies of earlier
research
For
example, Hirokawa claimed that when a group performs
critical functions, it reaches high-quality decisions. Hirokawa,
however, ignored the possibility that input variables might actually account
for his findings.
We can
consider some scenarios. For example, in one group, members are particularly
knowledgeable about how groups should make decisions. They believe they know
the correct steps. They realize they must understand both the dilemma itself
and the requirements for an acceptable solution to this dilemma. Further, they
understand that they should propose several realistic alternatives and assess
both the positive and the negative consequences of each alternative. In short,
the members know that groups should make decisions using the techniques that Hirokawa found at work in good groups. During their
discussion, each member of this group thinks individually and performs all
these "critical functions." Essentially, the members work on their own. They all come up with the same high-quality
decision because they have individually followed the same steps. In this case,
group discussion would have had little effect on the group's good decision.
In
contrast, the members of another group do not know much about how groups should
make decisions. Again, the members think through things individually. This
time, during group discussion, the members on their own each come up with the
same poor-quality decision. As in our first scenario, group discussion has had
little effect on decision quality. The poor decision comes out of the
individual processes of group members, not out of the processes that the group
goes through as a whole.
Hewes's Proposals
The importance of individual aspects. These two scenarios make up part
of what Hewes claimed actually happens during group
discussion. He suggested that the members' individual aspects determine the
quality of their group's decision. These individual aspects include the
members' personalities, motivations, knowledge, and decision-making skills. Hewes believed that what happens during group discussion
has little impact on the quality of a group's decision. In essence, Hewes believed that, if group members individually make
poor decisions, putting them together in a group will not help them to make a
better decision. Similarly, if people know how to make good decisions, a group
discussion will not hinder their abilities and lead them to a poor decision.
"Thinking aloud." If Hewes
is right, why have researchers such as Hirokawa found
a relationship between what happens during a group's discussion and the quality
of the decision that a group makes? Hewes has a ready
answer.
In Hewes's view, group members face two tasks when they engage
in group decision making: (1) participating in a discussion and (2) formulating
their own thoughts about the group decision. To complete the first task,
members need to make relevant comments in response to others. To complete the
second task, they must concentrate on the decision. Hewes's
idea was that when these two tasks come together they create some problems for
group members. He felt that people cannot simultaneously perform both these
tasks well. They have to let one fall by the wayside. Therefore, they
concentrate on the second task and focus their attention on thinking hard about
their decisions.
This
does not mean that people forget about their first task. They know they are
supposed to be talking with one another. To help themselves out of this dilemma,
they vocalize what they are thinking. In essence, their minds are filled with
their own thoughts, but they need to talk; so they end up thinking aloud. The
consequence is that group members speak but do not listen. Hewes
believed that group discussion consists of members thinking out loud but not
listening to one another.
"Vacuous acknowledgments." If Hewes
is correct, why does it seem to an outsider that group members react to one
another's statements? Hewes claimed that members say
things that make it seem as if they are seriously listening to one another. In
actuality, Hewes said, they are not. Members'
statements are "vacuous acknowledgments." Through the use of these
statements, members sound as if they are reacting to comments when in truth they
are thinking aloud. Hewes presented the following
example of how a member could use a vacuous acknowledgment (1986, p. 282):
Claude:
How about building an underpass for bikers on Sierra?
Steve: I
can see your point, but how about more money being put into traffic lights?
Steve's
statement "I can see your point, but" is a vacuous acknowledgment.
The acknowledgment makes it seem that Steve's comment is in response to Claude;
yet the actual content of Steve's statement has nothing to do with Claude's idea.
The proposals are completely different. In Hewes's
view, Steve is not really listening to Claude. Instead, he prefaces his own
"thinking out loud" with an acknowledgment that makes it appear that
he is responding to Claude. In reality, he is not. Hewes
claimed that an observer should not be surprised to see this pattern at work.
Group members say things to each other such as, "Yeah, and we could also .
. ." or "Right, but I've also been thinking about . . ." and
then proceed with comments that have nothing to do with what they supposedly
heard. Whenever members behave this way, they are doing what Hewes claimed group members normally do.
How
"good" groups appear "good." How could Hirokawa
have seen "good" groups if the members were mainly thinking aloud? Hewes's idea was that the individual behavior of members
can make a group appear "good." For instance, group members have the
same beliefs about how groups should make decisions. They all seek to follow
the same pattern. The members of this group are likely to think of the same
sorts of things at the same time. If they voice these thoughts, they would
appear to respond to one another when in actuality they are not.
Similarly,
imagine that the members individually are knowledgeable and skillful. Their
group is likely to make a good decision; however, group discussion is not an
important ingredient in that decision. The group would come out of the
discussion with a good decision because the members went into the meeting
knowing how to make good decisions on their own.
Actual challenge
We have
said that Hewes challenged the scientists who have
studied group discussion. In essence, he challenged them to prove his idea
wrong. To do so, scientists must show that group discussion consists of more
than vacuous acknowledgments and thinking out loud.
How can
researchers accomplish this? They need to demonstrate that during group
discussion members make comments that actually respond to the content of other
members' statements. Only in this way can they show that group members are
really working together and interacting, rather than ignoring one another's
ideas and merely thinking out loud. Further, they must show that the effect of
group discussion on group output is over and above the effect of input variables.
At
present, no study has succeeded in showing these two things. No study has
successfully isolated group discussion and made its effects independent of
group input. Therefore, as yet no study unambiguously demonstrates that group
discussion, independently, actually affects group output.
Several
studies have, however, tried to answer Hewes's
challenge.
Answers to Hewes's
Challenge
During the 1990s, Hirokawa has taken Hewes' challenge seriously. He has acknowledged the
possibility that group process may not have a significant impact on group
output. Hirokawa has not, however, come out totally
in favor of Hewes's standpoint. Rather, he has come
to believe that there are situations in which group process has no effect on
output. In contrast, there are other situations in which process is critical
for group performance. The job of the researcher is to determine when process
is and is not important.
Hirokawa
and his students have written several essays in which they have proposed
various input factors that affect the importance of group process in
determining group output. They have also performed some studies to evaluate
their proposals. Thus far, all of these studies have problems, and none have
succeeded in answering Hewes's challenge. We will
consider perhaps the best of these, performed by Hirokawa's
student Salazar.
Salazar Study
Salazar
proposed two input variables that he thought might impact the process/output
relationship. One of these was group
homogeneity. Salazar hypothesized that when group members have
similar beliefs or preferences, they find it relatively easy to reach
consensus, and this consensus will reflect those beliefs or preferences. As a
result, it will be those beliefs and not group process that will have the
largest impact on the group's decision. In contrast, when group members' prediscussional preferences are dissimilar, then they need
to communicate quite a bit to make a decision, and one cannot predict what the
decision will be based on those preferences. In this case, it is group process
and not prediscussional preferences that will impact
the group's decision.
The
second input variable Salazar considered was task
complexity. When tasks are simple, group members know how to
perform them, and need not spend much time discussing the issue. Thus group
process will not have a large effect on their performance. However, when tasks
are difficult, group members must interact to figure out how to do it. In this
circumstance, process takes a more critical role.
Method
In his
study, Salazar used the "Lost on the Moon" accuracy task that we
discussed in Chapter 2. As you recall, participants are asked to rank-order 15
items in terms of their survival value for a space crew stranded two hundred
miles from their mother ship. Participant rankings are then compared to those
made by survival experts. In the "low task complexity" conditions,
participants were asked to rank-order five of the items that experts had ranked
as very dissimilar in their survival value (those ranked 3, 6, 9, 12, and 15).
The researcher reasoned that group members would find it easy to judge the
relative value of each for the lost crew. In the "high task complexity
conditions," participants were asked to rank five items that were seen by
the experts to be very similar (11, 12, 13, 14, and 15). In this case, Salazar
thought that the participants would have a hard time distinguishing which of
the items were more or less important for survival.
Salazar
then formed groups that were either in prediscussional
agreement (high member homogeneity) or disagreement (low member homogeneity) to
perform either the high or low complex version of the "Lost on the
Moon" task. He content analyzed their communication using Hirokawa's (1982) coding scheme, in order to see which conditions
led to more or less of the "critical discussion functions." To
measure group output, he also evaluated the accuracy of each group's decision.
Findings
First,
Salazar analyzed the relationship between his input and process variables. He
found that groups with low member homogeneity performed more discussion of the
problem, the requirements of a good solution, and the positive and negative
consequences of alternative solutions than groups with high member homogeneity.
This supports his idea that a difficult task would lead to more relevant
discussion than a simple task. Task complexity, however, had very little impact
on critical discussion functions.
Salazar
then attempted to study the relationship between process and output. He found
that the accuracy of the member's prediscussional
preferences accounted fully for group accuracy when the groups were high in
homogeneity, no matter how complex the task. In these cases, communication had
no effect. In contrast, when the groups were low in homogeneity and the task
was complex, the accuracy of member prediscussional
preferences had no impact on group accuracy. This time, communication was
important. Finally, when groups were low in homogeneity and the task was
simple, both prediscussional preferences and
communication were important. However, each condition only included about ten
groups. This is not nearly enough for an accurate analysis, and these results
cannot be trusted.
Conclusion
Setting
aside the small number of groups, can Salazar's study be considered an answer
to Hewes's challenge? Not really. Hewes
insisted that researchers have to study the sequential structure of group
discussion. If there is no sequential structure, then there is no reason to
believe that group members are really responding to one another as they make
their decision. We have no way of knowing whether Salazar's participants are
really working with one another or whether they are just thinking aloud in one
another's presence.
Imagine
the following possibility: A non-homogeneous group is performing the complex
version of "Lost on the Moon." During the group meeting, each member
is thinking through the problem on their own out loud, not really listening to
one another. As they do so, they come to a better individual understanding of
it, and change their minds about the rank-ordering of the items. Their final
individual rankings are more accurate than their prediscussional
preferences, and so they "combine" these rankings into an accurate
group judgment. Their communication reflects their individual thought
processes, which makes it appear as if communication has led them to a more
accurate group answer. However, as they were never truly interacting,
communication is not really having any impact. As unlikely as this possibility
might appear, Salazar's findings cannot rule it out.
Despite
its methodological problems, and despite this latter possibility, there is no
question that Salazar's study is a serious attempt to show that group
discussion affects group output. We hope that future research overcomes these
difficulties and comes closer to a definitive answer to the question.
POSSIBLE PHASES IN GROUP
DISCUSSION
We now turn to the second general question about group process that has
intrigued scholars. This question concerns the manner in which group members
conduct their discussion.
Bales and the Phase Hypothesis
At times in this chapter we have discussed Bales's
contributions to group discussion theory and research. Now we shall examine his
phase hypothesis. To introduce this hypothesis, let us review what we have
already discussed about Bales's work.
Earlier
we explained Bales's theoretical views of the
equilibrium problem. We also described his research into the question of how
groups try to solve this dilemma. We then discussed Bales's
argument that groups must solve three additional problems before it can perform
its task.
The
first problem is that members must come to a common understanding, or orientation, toward the group's task.
Second, the group needs to create a common set of values, or evaluation, about an ideal solution.
Third, group members must use their influence and power relationships, or control, to come up with a good
solution. In essence, a group needs to find its own orientation, evaluation, and
control techniques before it can perform its task.
Further,
a group should solve these three problems in the order given. A group must come
to a common understanding before it can find a method of evaluation, and it
must have a common method of evaluation before it can choose the best solution.
As we
stated earlier, Bales argued that groups use discussion to solve the
equilibrium problem. He also believed that groups solve these three additional
problems through discussion. It follows then that different parts, or phases,
of the discussion should involve specific problems. The communication during
"phase one" should be concerned with
"orientation," for example, and so on. The group's discussion should
reflect these relationships.
This
line of reasoning is known as the "phase hypothesis" of group
discussion. To test this theory, Bales divided his group's discussions into
thirds. He then compared the relative amounts of acts in each third. His
results are shown in Table 8.5.
Table
8.5 |
|
Test
of Linear Phase Hypothesis |
|
|||
Rank |
Segment
1 |
|
Segment
2 |
|
Segment
3 |
|
|
Function |
Percent |
Function |
Percent |
Function |
Percent |
1 |
information |
35 |
opinion |
38 |
opinion |
35 |
2 |
opinion |
31 |
information |
25 |
pos.
reaction |
30 |
3 |
pos.
reaction |
20 |
pos.
reaction |
22 |
information |
19 |
4 |
suggestion |
6 |
neg.
reaction |
8 |
suggestion |
9 |
5 |
neg.
reaction |
6 |
suggestion |
7 |
neg.
reaction |
8 |
Bales
theorized that the group dealt with the problems of orientation, evaluation,
and control, in that order. The findings bear him out to some extent.
The
problem of orientation involves gaining a mutual understanding of the issue the
group is examining. It requires that members exchange relevant information
primarily during the first phase of discussion. As these results indicate,
Bales found that the first segment did contain a greater proportion of asking
for and giving information than did the second and third segments. This finding
was consistent with Bales's expectations.
To solve
the problem of evaluation, group members need to form mutual values for evaluating
proposed solutions. For example, if group members decide to make an outline,
they also need to agree on how they will know when the outline is good. This
requires that members share opinions concerning these values. As evaluation is
the second of the three problems, asking for and giving opinions should be
proportionately higher in that section than in any other. The data show that
opinions were highest in the third section, but not by very much.
The
problem of control requires that the group reach a consensus on the best
solution. Group participants suggest solutions to solve the problem. Thus
asking for and giving suggestions should be at their highest proportional level
during the third segment. Again, this was the case, but only by a very tiny margin.
As we
can also see in Table 8.5, the total number of positive and negative
maintenance messages went up from segment to segment. Bales concluded from this
finding that as the group progresses on its task, its tension
level goes up and more negative maintenance statements occur. To
counteract these, the group responds with more positive maintenance. Although
it is not shown in Table 8.5, Bales also found more subtle evidence concerning
negative and positive messages in the control phase. He found that, during the
first half of the third segment, groups exhibited a larger proportion of
negative reactions than they did during the second half of the third segment.
The proportions of positive reactions fell and rose accordingly. This finding
implies that the group has a great deal of conflict during the first half of
the control phase, and resolves the conflict during the second half. It
warrants dividing the control phase into two subphases,
negative and positive.
Additional Research
Bales's
contributions are among the largest of any scholar in the history of
small-group research. First and foremost should have been his theoretical work.
The field has largely ignored his accomplishments in that area, however.
Instead, Bales's research methods and findings,
divorced from the theory they intended to evaluate, have proved far more
influential.
Bales's
phase hypothesis has been the most well known part of his work. His coding
scheme, however, has had almost as much impact on research. In the 1950s and
early 1960s scores of studies retested the phase hypothesis. Many employed Bales's coding scheme despite the fact that he designed it
specifically to test his theory. Its relevance to other studies would therefore
be questionable.
Tuckman
(1965) reviewed this additional research. He concluded that groups pass through
four phases, with each phase containing both maintenance and task phases that
run roughly concurrently. Hence, four maintenance phases and four task phases
occur together. The phases are as follows:
Phase One--Forming
Maintenance - "Testing and dependence"-discovery of the
limits of acceptable behaviors during interaction.
Task -
"Orientation"-attempts to define the task and the manner in which the
group will accomplish it.
Phase Two--Storming
Maintenance - "Development of intragroup
conflicts"-polarization around interpersonal issues, resistance to
emerging structure of interpersonal relationships.
Task - "Emotional response to task demands"-resistance to
attempts at influence, conflict between individual orientations and task
demands.
Phase Three--Norming
Maintenance - "Development of group cohesion"-acceptance of
a structure for interpersonal relationships.
Task - "Open exchange of relevant
interpretations"-expression of opinions dealing with the problem.
Phase Four--Performing
Maintenance - "Functional role-relatedness"-use of the
now-agreed-upon relationship structure to solve interpersonal problems.
Task - "Emergence of solutions"-constructive attempts to
complete the task.
Tuckman's analysis implies that a group's social structure is important for
the task. The degree to which a group's social structure has evolved determines
the group's ability to perform its task at each phase. The social structure
serves as a foundation for the performance of tasks.
The Linear Phase Model of Group
Process
Bales
and his immediate successors left the legacy of the "phase
hypothesis" for future researchers. We have shown how this hypothesis
relates to the process of group discussion. The theory has come to be known as
the "linear phase" model of group discussion because it implies that
groups handle problems sequentially. A group will go through each prerequisite
stage before starting subsequent stages. It also will never go back to earlier
stages.
The
linear phase model may be valid in the area of concern that scientists such as
Bales and Tuckman researched. They were interested in
the function of interaction in the development of a group's social and task
structures. The linear model is deficient as a complete hypothesis, however.
Later theorists turned their attention to the development of specific ideas
under discussion in a group. As they did so, it became apparent that the linear
phase model was insufficient for characterizing group process. Nevertheless, keep Bales's work in mind as we
continue to examine theories concerning group process.
Scheidel and Crowell
A coding
scheme for interaction analysis is like a scientific perspective. Both point
out certain features of the "real world" but hide others. We can see
this in Bales's coding. His coding scheme points out
the functions of statements in group discussion but hides the manner in which
specific ideas develop over the course of a discussion.
Scheidel
and Crowell (1964) were among the first scientists to escape the domination of Bales's influence. They proposed the following coding
scheme.
1 -
Initiation of new idea |
7P -
Positive modification |
2 -
Restatement |
7C -
Negative modification |
3 -
Clarification |
8A -
Stated agreement |
4 -
Substantiation |
8B -
Stated disagreement |
5 -
Extension of idea |
9 -
Synthesis |
6 -
Minor response |
10 -
Summary |
Q -
Question |
|
As you
can see, Scheidel and Crowell included only task
functions. The maintenance functions of acts are absent from the chart. Thus,
the equilibrium hypothesis is invisible from the view this scheme implies. In
addition, Scheidel and Crowell instructed the coders
to note the proposal that each act, or statement, covered. This allowed them to
trace how proposals developed during the discussion. Bales did not do this.
Research results
In their
major research, Scheidel and Crowell used their
coding scheme to analyze the discussion of five groups. Each group consisted of
four to seven members.
At the
first level, the level of acts, approximately one-fourth of the conversation
involved initiating, extending, modifying, or synthesizing proposals. In other
words, one-fourth was creative work. The second fourth dealt with clarifying
and substantiating proposals. A third fourth consisted of evaluating proposals.
The researchers did not mention the final fourth. We presume that it was
maintenance oriented and thus irrelevant to the scientists' analysis for this
study.
More
important, researchers discovered much about the sequential, or interact,
structure of the discussions. Statements that clarified/substantiated or
evaluated usually followed communication acts that "created"
proposals. These researchers called this tendency "reach-testing."
Group members first followed the process of "reaching out," in which
they initiated a new proposal or developed an old proposal. Groups then seemed
to need to "test" their ability to accept the proposal by clarifying
it or evaluating it. If the result of the test was affirmative, the proposal became
an "anchor." The group could then go through another "reaching
out" by using the "anchor" as a base.
If this
process continued, one member's initiation became progressively modified until
slowly it represented the entire group's point of view. In many cases, however,
the idea received a negative evaluation. The group then returned to a previous
anchor and reached out in a different direction.
Spiral model
In many
instances groups reintroduce proposals that they had previously discussed. Scheidel and Crowell emphasized this finding in their
discussion. They characterized this finding as indicative of a "spiral
model" of group process. This model opposed the linear model in two ways.
First, the linear model maintains that groups never look back. Groups should
move in a linear pattern and follow a continued progression through stages of
discussion. The spiral model allows the group to look backward.
This
difference is not really relevant, however. Scheidel
and Crowell found spiraling in the ways that groups developed their proposals.
Bales and Tuckman saw a linear progression in the
ways that groups developed their social and task structures. As we can see, the
researchers were looking at two different features of group discussion. The
findings do not necessarily conflict.
Bales's
second interact diagram, showing contiguous statements by different speakers,
illustrates how groups use statements of agreement following task work. Bales
considered statements of agreement maintenance acts. In this way, his diagram
shows a tendency that is consistent with the equilibrium hypothesis. Scheidel and Crowell, on the other hand, thought that
statements of agreement were task acts in themselves. For them, the statements
of agreement showed reach-testing. Thus, both research programs found similar
results. They both saw that statements of agreement followed statements that
were task oriented. They interpreted these results differently, however.
Second,
the linear models of Bales and of Tuckman limit the
time when the group works on the problem. Bales said that such work takes place
in later stages, after the group has gone through orientation and evaluation
stages. Scheidel and Crowell's results show that the
group works on its problem throughout the discussion.
In this
case, Scheidel and Crowell's interpretation is
justified. We can reexamine Bales's data on phases.
The amounts of each type of statement across the three
segments is important. These amounts are too similar to support the
conclusion that task work is not being performed throughout the discussion.
Fisher
We have shown two models for group process from the functionalist perspective:
(1) the linear model of group structural development and (2) the spiral model
of group idea development. These models address different group functions, and
each has merits and deficiencies. The potential exists, however, to synthesize
a model that contains elements of both hypotheses. B. A. Fisher (1974) proposed
a synthesis.
As did Scheidel and Crowell, Fisher used a coding scheme that
focused on the task functions of communicative acts (see Appendix for a list of
its categories). Fisher's scheme classifies acts according to function:
interpretation, substantiation, clarification, modification, and evaluation. It
also classifies them as favorable, unfavorable, or ambiguous toward the idea
under consideration. Further, it is important, as with Scheidel
and Crowell, that coders trace the progression of specific ideas during the
discussion.
Fisher's
own research supported the synthesis of linear and spiral models. He examined
groups of four to seven people who met for 25 minutes to 30 hours. His work
suggested a four-phase sequence. Positive statements were greatest in all
phases. Differences occurred, however, in the relative number of ambiguous and
negative acts.
During
the first phase, the "orientation" segment, ambiguous statements were
at their relative highest, almost as plentiful as the positive acts. During the
second phase, the "conflict" segment, negativity was at its maximum
point, and ambiguous statements fell to their minimum level. Further, analysis
of the interacts showed that in this segment the
tendency for conflict among the four phases was greatest. By conflict we mean
periods when statements for and against an idea alternated.
During
the third segment, the "emergence" phase, ambiguity reemerged,
although it was still not as high as during the orientation phase. In addition,
negativity dropped. One proposal seemed to win. Dissenters no longer objected
to proposals they didn't like. Instead, their responses became noncommittal. In
the fourth phase, the "reinforcement" segment, positive statements
rose to their highest levels. They outnumbered ambiguous and negative acts by
the largest proportion of all phases. The groups had achieved consensus;
however, rather than immediately adjourning, the groups took time to express
support for the chosen proposal. (Note the similarity of Fisher's findings with
those of Hoffman in Chapter 7.)
Thus, as
we can see, Fisher's work supported a linear model of idea evaluation.
Scientists could apply the linear concept to ideas as well as to task acts.
Fisher found that spiral "reach-testing" also occurred in the groups.
He saw this as he traced specific ideas through the conversation. Even when a
group seemed to support an idea, the group often dropped it. The group would
then turn to another proposal for a while. Then, once again it would
reintroduce the idea it seemed to support and discuss it further, often in a
modified form.
In the
end, Fisher's work supported aspects of both the linear and the spiral models.
In this way he synthesized the two.
The work of
Criticisms of the four-phase
model
First,
the research had made arbitrary "segments." Many earlier studies had
divided group interaction into segments that corresponded to the theoretical
stages of the model. This action limited the number of "stages" that
the researcher could find. For example, if a researcher divides interaction
into four segments, he or she can find only four or fewer phases. The researcher
cannot find five or more stages even if they exist. The segmentation dictates
the research from the beginning. Researchers could not find more subtle changes
in interaction than they had proposed because they initially divided their data
into four stages.
Second,
most studies combined the data from many groups before they analyzed it. Groups
may be so different that researchers should not do this. If scientists look at
the groups only as a whole, they may be unable to make valid generalizations
about any one group. Any differences among groups might cancel out when
researchers combine data from all of them. This situation is similar to when we
talk about the "average person"--differences among people are
ignored. All scientists, however, know that the "average person"
concept is only a convenient fiction, not representing even one real person.
Individual groups may be as different as individual people. If so, then
researchers should keep this in mind. If they insist on combining data from
different groups, then they must speak of the "average group." They
should admit that the "average group" is a fiction just like the
"average person." It may be a convenient concept, but it may not
represent any group in reality.
Research
In more
recent research,
Further,
the researchers found that groups that most often followed a pattern like the
linear phase model had certain traits. These groups tended to be relatively
large, with six or more members, and have low cohesiveness and a diffused power
structure. In addition, they often performed tasks that had no clear goals and
many possible solutions. In contrast, the groups that most often ignored
problem analysis entirely had different characteristics. Such groups tended to
be relatively small, with three to five members, and have high cohesiveness and
a centralized power structure. Further, they often performed tasks that had
clear goals and only a few possible solutions.
These
findings support the idea that group members have a concept of how groups
"should" make decisions and that their concept is like the linear
phase model. It also appears that members believe that their group needs to
follow this model only when it will be difficult for the group to reach a
decision. In essence, the members try to follow the model when they feel they
need it, but not otherwise. For instance, it might be hard to reach a decision
when a group is uncoordinated or its task is difficult. Members of this group
will probably want to use their concept of "ideal" discussion. On the
other hand, it is not so difficult to reach a decision when a group is
coordinated or the task is easy. People in this kind of group feel that it is
unnecessary to analyze the problem. They believe they can go right into
discussing possible solutions, and they do not follow their concept of
"ideal" discussion.
"Tracks"
As you
will remember, Tuckman believed that the development
in social process serves as the foundation for the growth in task process.
Tracks
may, however, develop at the same rate. Such parallel evolution gives the
illusion of consistent phases and approximates earlier models of group process.
This convergence of parallel growth may represent an "ideal" type of
group process. Perhaps groups should attempt to meet it. This idea awaits further theoretical and research advances.
Pavitt
As we have discussed,
To find
out how people think groups "should" work,
the study focuses on the linear and spiral models of group discussion. The
research looks at how the two models relate and asks a specific question. We
will come to this question by first reviewing what we know about the models.
The
linear model suggests that a group will perform an entire phase of the process
before it moves to the next phase. The group leaves no stage unfinished to come
back to later. Thus, group members ought to finish the task of proposing all
possible solutions before they move on to the phase in which they discuss them.
Similarly, they should discuss all solutions before they evaluate any of them.
The linear model may come closest to the "ideal" method for making a
decision. Nevertheless,
Research
by Scheidel and Crowell and Fisher showed that groups
often perform "reach-testing" sequences as they discuss proposals. In
other words, group members often suggest, discuss, and evaluate one proposal
before suggesting another. They go through each "phase" several
times, each time they look at a proposal. They do not move in a steady
progression during a meeting.
Thus,
the specific research question of the study attempts to discover why groups
"spiral" so often when scientists think that a linear process is
better.
To
answer this question, the study sought to find out what conceptions people have
about ideal group procedure. What models appealed to the group members
themselves? The participants' task was to imagine that they were members of a
group that is making an important decision. Members would make and discuss
three proposals during their discussion. Each participant then received 15
cards. On each card was a description of a possible step in the group discussion
process. Participants received the cards in a random order. The task of each
participant was to place the cards in the order in which they would want their
group to step through the discussion process.
We had
expectations about some of the cards. Two cards read "Define Problem"
and "Establish Group Goal." We believed that most participants would
place the "Define Problem" cards first and the "Establish Group
Goal" card second, which they did in almost every case. Thus, these two
cards represent the "beginning" of the discussion.
Other
cards fit the study's expectations: "Eliminate Bad Solutions,"
"Improve Solutions," "Choose Best Solution," and
"Improve Best Solution." We believed that these four would be the
last cards in the participants' orderings. Although the specific ordering of
these cards differed substantially, far more often than not, they generally
were the last four. Hence, these four cards represent the "end" of
the discussion.
The
other nine cards read as follows:
Propose
First Solution |
Clarify
First Solution |
Evaluate
First Solution |
Propose
Second Solution |
Clarify
Second Solution |
Evaluate
Second Solution |
Propose
Third Solution |
Clarify
Third Solution |
Evaluate
Third Solution |
If a
study participant thinks that groups ought to follow a linear process when
making a decision, his or her ordering should reflect that. The order of cards
would be: the two "beginning" cards, all three "propose"
cards, all three "clarify" cards, all three "evaluate" cards,
and then the four "end" cards. The cards will be in a different order
if, instead, a participant thinks that groups ought to follow a spiral process.
In such a case, the ordering would be: the two "beginning" cards, all
three cards (propose, clarify, and evaluate) relating to the first solution,
all three cards that relate to the second solution, all three cards that relate
to the third solution, and finally the four "end" cards.
These
two example orderings are extremely faithful to their models, the linear phase
model and the spiral model. People can however "compromise." For
example, a participant could believe a group should "spiral" between
possible solutions as they propose and clarify them but wait to evaluate them
all together in one stage. On the other hand is a participant who feels that
groups should propose all possible solutions in one stage but then spiral
between the solutions when clarifying and evaluating them.
The
study results showed that about one-half of the participants ordered their
cards to reflect the spiral model. We called these participants
"reach-testers." Only one-fifth ordered their cards to reflect the
linear phase model. We named these "linears."
The remaining three-tenths created one or another compromise. That so few
participants chose to order their cards in linear phases might explain why
actual group discussion so often fails to meet this model.
This
study is continuing and is now looking at the way the participants' group
discussions progress. The question is whether each discussion actually follows
the card orderings of the members of that group. Do the groups behave as the
participating members felt they "ought" to? If so, we would expect
that groups consisting of linears would use a linear
process and that groups consisting of reach-testers would use a spiral process.
We also want to find out what happens in even-sized groups that contain half linears and half reach-testers. What process will these
groups use? Will the linears and the reach-testers
overtly conflict about the correct process? Will the way they
"choose" a process reveal anything interesting about power and social
influence? We hope the research will reveal preliminary answers to these and
other questions about group process.
SUMMARY
Researchers have studied group process from the viewpoints of two perspectives:
the functional and the interactional.
Functionalists
are concerned with the ways groups complete their tasks and maintain their
cohesiveness. Functional researchers explore the aspects of group process that
help or hinder these two aspects of groups. Scientists have proposed several
functional approaches toward group process. We can view these approaches as
existing on levels of abstraction. As a result, they are not necessarily
inconsistent.
Interactional researchers, in contrast, are concerned with discussion patterns.
They look at the types of statements that occur during group discussion and
attempt to describe their sequential structure.
Overall,
research in group process has been concerned with two major areas: the relationship
between group process and output, and the way groups utilize types of
statements during group discussion.
Bales has done extensive research in how group process relates to group
output. His findings suggest that groups need to do certain things if they wish
simultaneously to maintain their cohesiveness and solve their task problems.
Bales looked at statements on a low level of abstraction that perform
maintenance and task functions. He found that groups need to balance the
proportion and sequence of these statements if they wish to do well. In
contrast, Hirokawa found a set of "critical
functions" on a high level of abstraction. He discovered that decision
quality is associated with the performance of these functions. Hewes challenged the findings of both Bales and Hirokawa. Hewes argued that
neither study showed conclusive evidence that group process, and not group
input variables, relate to group output. Research has begun to answer Hewes's challenge.
In the
second major area, concerning how group members use statements, studies of
group process have examined how discussion progresses. The question has been
whether groups change the proportion of types of statements as they go along.
Scientists call this idea the "linear phase model." Bales hypothesized
that groups must go through a series of three linear stages. Each phase has
different proportions of varying types of statements.
Later
research has found that the linear phase model is too simple an explanation.
For example, cyclical processes occur as a group discusses specific proposals.
This process is explained in the "spiral model." Groups may abandon
an idea but then go back to it. Such action is not linear. Other research also
found large disparities in the number and order of phases that individual
groups go through during discussion. Research continues in an attempt to
discover when group discussion will and will not follow the pattern of the
linear phase model.